Tongue Wagging

When the Mars Rover landed on Mars, it bounced several times on balloon‑like cushions; the cushions then deflated, allowing the rover to roll gently onto the iron‑red dirt. If you had been there watching the bouncy landing, you would have heard–as you writhed in pain from decompression in the low‑pressure atmosphere–a sequence of hits, with rings in between. And once the rover found a place to take a sample of Martian soil, it would have scraped debris into a container for analysis, and that scrape would have sounded like a slide, followed by a ring characteristic of the Rover’s scraping arm. Hits, slides, and rings on Mars! It is not so much that hits, slides, and rings are Earthly nature’s phonemes as much as they are physics’ phonemes. These sounds are the principal building blocks of event sounds anywhere there are solid objects interacting–even in our mouths.

Our mouths have moving parts, including a powerful and acrobatic tongue; fleshy, maneuverable lips; and a jaw rigged with rock‑hard teeth. When we speak, these parts physically interact in complex ways, creating speech events. But speech events are events , and if hits, slides, and rings are the fundamental constituents of physical events, then speech events must also be built from hits, slides, and rings in the mouth. It is no wonder, then, that human speech sounds like hits, slides, and rings. Speech is built from the fundamental constituents of physical events because speech is a physical event. Harnessing would appear to have nothing to do with it.

However, when we speak, our mouth is not simply a container with a tongue, lips, and teeth rattling around. We are not, for example, making hit sounds by tapping our teeth together, or slide sounds by grinding our teeth. When our mouth (in collaboration with our nose, throat, and lungs) makes sounds, it is using mechanisms for sound production that go well beyond the solid‑object event atoms–hits, slides, and rings. Although hits, slides, and rings are the most fundamental kinds of physical events (because solid‑object events are the most fundamental kind of physical event), they are not the only kinds. There are hosts of others. In particular, there are many physical events that involve the flow of fluid or air. The events in our mouths that make the sounds of speech are events involving airflow, not hits, slides, or rings at all. Airflow events in our mouths mimic hits, slides, and rings, the constituents of solid‑object physical events. Our mouths make a plosive by a sudden release of air, not by an actual collision in the mouth. Fricatives are made by the noninstantaneous movement of air through a tight passage; no surfaces in the mouth are actually rubbed against one another. And sonorants are not due to an object vibrating because of a hit or slide; instead, sonorants come from the vocal chords vibrating as air passes by.

Hit, slide, and ring sounds without hits, slides, or rings! What a coincidence! Human speech employs three principal sounds via airflow mechanisms, and yet they happen to sound just like the three principal sounds that happen in events with physical interactions between solid objects. Utterly different mechanisms, but the same resultant sound. That’s too coincidental to be a coincidence. That’s just what harnessing expects: airflow sound‑producing mouths settling on just a few sounds for language–the sounds of physical interactions among solid objects.

We must be careful, though. What if airflow mechanisms cannot help but make hit, slide, and ring sounds? Or, more to the point, could it be that the particular airflow mechanisms our mouths are capable of can lead only to sounds like hits, slides, and rings? No. Human mouths are capable of sounds much more varied than the sounds of interacting solid objects. For example, people can mimic many animal sounds–quacks, moos, barks, ribbits, meows, and even human sounds like slurps, burps, sneezes, and yawns–that are constructed out of constituents beyond simple hit, slide, and ring sounds. People can mimic water‑related sounds–like splashes, flushes, and drips–none of which are built from hit, slide, and ring sounds. And our airflow sound‑mimicking mouths can, of course, mimic airflow sounds–like a soda pop being opened, howling wind, or even breaking wind–also unrelated to the sounds of hits, slides, and rings. People can mimic “hot” sounds, like sizzling bacon and roaring fires. They can even mimic the sounds of revving motorcycles, fax machines, digital alarm clocks, shrilling phones, and alien spaceships, none of which are sounds built from hits, slides, and rings. We see, then, that our airflow sound‑producing mouths have a very wide repertoire, and yet speech has employed only the barest of our talents for mimicry, preferring exactly the sounds that occur among interacting macroscopic solid objects. We’re not, therefore, speaking in hits, slides, and rings by default. That we find these in all languages is a sign that we have been harnessed.

In upcoming sections, I will also concentrate on some other kinds of sounds our mouths can produce, but that language tends to avoid; these cases deserve special attention because of their prima facie similarity to sounds we do find in speech. Thus, they can help to answer the question of why speech utilizes some sounds we can make, but not others we can make just as easily. For example, we will see in the upcoming section that although we can make the sounds of wiggly hits and slides, we do not have them as phonemes–and this is consistent with their absence in physics. In the section following that we will see that although we can make slide‑hit sounds and hit‑slide sounds, only the latter is given the honor of phoneme status in languages (see the section titled “Nature’s Other Phoneme”), consistent with hit‑slides being a fundamental sound in physics, while slide‑hits are not. And we’ll see in the “Two‑Hit Wonder” section that a simple kind of sound (a “beep”) that could exist as a phoneme does not occur in human languages, consistent with its nonprimitive status in physics. More generally, for the next five sections I will brandish a magnifying glass and closely examine the internal structures of hits, slides, and rings, asking whether those same fine structures are found in plosives, fricatives, and sonorants, respectively.

 








Дата добавления: 2015-05-08; просмотров: 733;


Поиск по сайту:

При помощи поиска вы сможете найти нужную вам информацию.

Поделитесь с друзьями:

Если вам перенёс пользу информационный материал, или помог в учебе – поделитесь этим сайтом с друзьями и знакомыми.
helpiks.org - Хелпикс.Орг - 2014-2024 год. Материал сайта представляется для ознакомительного и учебного использования. | Поддержка
Генерация страницы за: 0.004 сек.